Children with ADHD make
more incorrect decisions on when to begin crossing a street than children
without ADHD
Children with attention deficit/hyperactivity
disorder (ADHD) are at increased risk of being hit by a vehicle
when crossing a street, according to new research from the University
of Alabama at Birmingham.
The findings, published July 25, 2011, in Pediatrics, indicate
that children with ADHD don't process information as well as non-ADHD
children and tend to make incorrect decisions on when to begin crossing
a street to a greater extent than non-ADHD children. According to
the National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, one of the
leading causes of unintentional injury in middle childhood is pedestrian
injury.
"The kids with ADHD in our study displayed the behaviors parents
want to see - they stopped at the street and looked both ways. But
that doesn't mean they are ready to cross a street by themselves,"
said the study's first author, Despina Stavrinos, Ph.D., assistant
professor in the UAB Injury Control Research Center.
As a result, the researchers suggest parents of children with ADHD
may want to delay the time when their children are allowed to cross
a street by themselves.
The UAB study involved 78 children ages 7 to 10 years old, 39 with
ADHD and 39 without. The children completed 10 simulated street
crossings in UAB's Youth Safety Laboratory, which houses a virtual
street environment. The simulator shows a typical street scene,
with vehicles approaching on monitors from both left and right.
Children are asked to gauge the proper moment to safely cross the
virtual street and then step off the curb, initiating the cross.
Stavrinos said children with ADHD did the right things when approaching
a street - waiting to cross and looking left and right before crossing
- in a manner similar to the non-ADHD control children.
"However, at some point in the decision-making process, things
appear to go awry, resulting in a dangerous crossing environment,"
Stavrinos said. "It seems children with ADHD are attempting
to properly assess the environment's safety, but are failing to
process the information in a manner that enables them to cross safely."
The children with ADHD picked shorter gaps between oncoming traffic,
had more "close-calls" with traffic and a shorter amount
of time left to spare upon reaching the other end of the crosswalk.
Stavrinos suggests the cause may be executive functioning, a term
that describes the processes by which the brain controls behavior.
Poor executive function has previously been identified in children
with ADHD. Executive function includes aspects such as timing, inhibition
and planning and executing action.
"Proper executive functioning would entail recognizing the
speed of the oncoming vehicle, the interval between vehicles and
the speed of the walker as they cross the street," Stavrinos
said. "Children with ADHD seem to be behind their typically
developing peers in these sorts of computing skills."
Stavrinos says continued practice might be valuable in teaching
the child with ADHD how to recognize a safe gap in traffic.
Collaborators on the study include Fred Biasini, Ph.D.; David Schwebel,
Ph.D.; Philip Fine, Ph.D., MSPH; J. Bart Hodgens, Ph.D.; Snehal
Khatri, M.D.; and Sylvie Mrug, Ph.D.
The study was supported by the Society for Public Health Education,
National Science Foundation, Center for Child Injury Prevention
Studies, Society for Pediatric Psychology and UAB Injury Control
Research Center.
|