New insights into emotional memories
suggest that sleep plays a key role in determining what we remember - and what
we forget
Sleep helps the brain selectively preserve and enhance
those aspects of a memory that are of greatest emotional resonance, while at the
same time diminishing the memory's neutral background details according to a study
published in the August 2008 issue of the journal Psychological Science.
"This tells us that sleep's role in emotional memory
preservation is more than just mechanistic," says the study's first author Jessica
Payne, PhD, a Harvard University research fellow in the Division of Psychiatry
at BIDMC. "In order to preserve what it deems most important, the brain makes
a tradeoff, strengthening the memory's emotional core and obscuring its neutral
background."
Previous studies have established the key role that sleep
plays in procedural memory, demonstrating that the consolidation of procedural
skills (such as typing or playing the piano) is greatly enhanced following a period
of sleep. But sleep's importance in the development of episodic memories - in
particular, those with emotional resonance- has been less clear.
"Emotional memories usually contain highly charged elements
- for example, the car that sideswiped us on the ride home - along with other
elements that are only tangentially related to the emotion, such as the name of
the street we were traveling on or what store we'd just passed," explains study
author Elizabeth Kensinger, PhD, an Assistant Professor in the College of Arts
and Sciences at Boston College. "We were interested in examining whether sleep
would affect memory for all of these elements equally, or whether sleep might
allow some of the event features to decay at a faster rate than others."
The authors tested 88 college students. Study participants
were shown scenes that depicted either neutral subjects on a neutral background
(a car parked on a street in front of shops) or negatively arousing subjects on
a neutral background (a badly crashed car parked on a similar street). The participants
were then tested separately on their memories of both the central objects in the
pictures and the backgrounds in the scenes. In this way, memory could be compared
for the emotional aspects of a scene (the crashed car) versus the non-emotional
aspects of the scene (the street on which the car had crashed.)
Subjects were divided into three groups. The first group
underwent memory testing after 12 hours spent awake during the daytime; the second
group was tested after 12 nighttime hours, including their normal period of nighttime
sleep; and the third baseline group was tested 30 minutes after viewing the images,
in either the morning or evening.
"Our results revealed that the study subjects who stayed
awake all day largely forgot the entire negative scene [they had seen], with their
memories of both the central objects and the backgrounds decaying at similar rates,"
says Payne. But, she adds, among the individuals who were tested after a period
of sleep, memory recall for the central negative objects (i.e. the smashed car)
was preserved in detail.
"After an evening of sleep, the subjects remembered the
emotional items [smashed car] as accurately as the subjects whose memories had
been tested only 30 minutes after looking at the scenes," explains Kensinger.
"By contrast, sleep did little to preserve memory for the backgrounds [i.e. street
scenes] and so memory for those elements reached a comparably low level after
a night of sleep as it did after a day spent awake."
"This is consistent with the possibility that the individual
components of emotional scene memory become 'unbound' during sleep," adds Payne,
explaining that "unbinding" enables the sleeping brain to selectively preserve
only that information which it calculates to be most salient and worthy of remembering.
A real-world example of this tradeoff, she adds, is the "weapon focus effect"
in which crime victims vividly remember an assailant's weapon, but have little
memory for other important aspects of the crime scene. Traumatic memories, such
as the flashbacks experienced among individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder,
can demonstrate similar disparities, with some aspects of an experience seemingly
engraved in memory while other details are erased.
"Sleep is a smart, sophisticated process," adds Payne.
"You might say that sleep is actually working at night to decide what memories
to hold on to and what to let go of."
This study was supported, in part, by grants from the
National Science Foundation and the National Institute of Mental Health. Coauthors
include Elizabeth Kensinger, PhD, of Boston College, Robert Stickgold, PhD, of
Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center; and Kelley Swanberg of Harvard University.
|